Teaching the Meaning
of Numbers
How to close the achievement gap in math? Tisch Lecturer Robert Siegler
talks about Chutes and Ladders and other strategies
It’s accepted economic wisdom that the rich get richer, and
the poor get poorer. But as Dr. Robert Sigler, Teresa Heinz Professor of
Cognitive Psychology at Carnegie Mellon University, pointed out at TC’s 2010
Tisch Lecture, that statement applies to the accumulation of mathematical
understanding as well. That is: students who enter a classroom with greater
proficiency in math can grow their knowledge base at faster rates than those
with lesser proficiency.
A prolific researcher and author, Siegler – TC’s Tisch
Visiting Professor for 2009-10
-- has devoted much of his career to understanding children’s
thinking, particularly around mathematical and scientific concepts and
particularly within low-income populations.
By age four, there are clearly recognizable differences
between wealthier and poorer children in understanding of object counting, next
number, number comparison, two set addition, shape names and other math-related
skills.
“If we had the educational system we’d all like, we’d still
have different levels of knowledge,” Siegler told his Milbank Chapel audience
on February 23rd, “but the difference would be mitigated with schooling, or at
least would get no larger. But in the U.S., differences increase with
schooling.”
In the U.S., the learning gap between low- and middle-income
groups is typically one and a half years in the first grade. By the time
students reach the ninth grade that gap has grown to three years, a point at
which the disparities seem insurmountable.
According to Siegler, preschool is the best time to try to
break that pattern, partly because the gap is still relatively small, and
partly because establishing a base of skills and knowledge at that point
creates “cognitive multipliers” for later on. Again, the rich get richer – or,
in this case, the more you know, the easier it is to keep learning.
Of course, a student can learn to count to ten by rote memorization
or learning a song, but that doesn’t necessarily translate into a genuine understanding
of whether 4 is smaller than 6, and why. How well children understand such
“numerical magnitudes” correlates strongly with how they perform on achievement
tests, Siegler says.
So how to teach numerical magnitudes in the classroom?
One answer advocated by Siegler and colleagues: numerical
board games. For example, Chutes and Ladders, a game that traces its roots back
to ancient India, has proven especially effective at exposing low- and
middle-income preschoolers to what Siegler calls “rich multimodal data,” such
as verbal, visual-spatial, kinesthetic, auditory and temporal cues to numerical
magnitudes.
In a 2008 study, Siegler compared the effectiveness of Chutes
and Ladders to that of another, color-based board game as a tool to help
students learn about the meaning behind numbers. Post-intervention tests
recorded significant gains for the groups who worked with numerical board
games.
Two other studies—both in 2009—looked at game playing
outside of the lab in low-income preschoolers, seeking to identify which
physical features (linear or circular) of the games were essential to numerical
learning. In all this work, Siegler and his colleagues arrivied at the same
conclusion: early experiences with numerical concepts help all preschool kids,
but they help the low-income ones the most. Thus, briefly-targeted
interventions can go a long way in reducing knowledge disparities between low-
and middle-income groups—and, in the words of Siegler, toward “leveling the
playing field.”
At the end of Siegler’s lecture, one audience member stood
and said: “That motivated me to want to go out and encourage thousands of
children to go play board games.”
Published Wednesday, Mar. 10, 2010